Skip to main content
Log in

Can a Scientist Be a Young, Attractive Woman? The Stereotype of a Scientist and the Lexical Choices of Women and Men

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The question of the conformance of a researcher’s features to the stereotype of a scientist is rarely addressed in the context of scientific research. We decided to examine its significance in two experiments involving women and men in which the persons conducting the experiment had features respectively conforming and not conforming to the stereotype of a scientist. Both experiments were carried out on an interactive model and the dependent variables were length of utterance and lexical choices. We chose to use linguistic material because, as classical research shows, it is particularly susceptible to the influence of social context and features of the interlocutor. To operationalise the dependent variable, we used Ertel’s Speech Style Quotients. The results of both experiments were found to be significant for context comparisons but non-significant for gender, which confirms the importance of features of the interlocutor in determining utterance length and lexical choices.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in Open Science Framework at https:/osf.io, reference number zj79s.

Notes

  1. The number of participants in the second experiment is given in parentheses.

  2. All of the competent judges involved in constructing the method and the research tools were post-doctoral research staff of Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań.

  3. A detailed description of the Polish adaptation of the quotients can be found in Obrębska (2013).

  4. The results on differences in utterance length for the first experiment were published by Obrębska and Kleka (2016a).

  5. The results on lexical choices for the first experiment were published by Obrębska and Kleka (2016b).

  6. Because the results for AQ in the first experiment did not satisfy normality assumptions, the ANOVA result was confirmed by a non-parametric Kruksal–Wallis test (chi2(5) = 15.8, p = .007).

References

  • Adler, R. B., Rosenfeld, L. B., & Proctor, R. F. (2003). Interplay: The process of interpersonal communication. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aries, E. (1996). Men and women in interaction. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aries, E. (2006). Gender differences in interaction: A reexamination. In D. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex and gender differences in communication (2nd ed., pp. 65–81). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banchefsky, S., Westfall, J., Park, B., & Judd, C. M. (2016). But you don’t look like a scientist!: Women scientists with feminine appearance are deemed less likely to be scientists. Sex Roles, 75, 95–109. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0586-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, T. X. (1973). Pitfalls in research: Nine investigator and experimenter effects. In R. M. W. Travers (Ed.), Second handbook of research on teaching (pp. 382–404). Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Interpersonal effects of experimenter attractiveness, attire, and gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(2), 435–446. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.435

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanck, P. D., Rosenthal, R., Snodgrass, S. E., DePaulo, B. M., & Zuckerman, M. (1981). Sex differences in eavesdropping on nonverbal cues: Developmental changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 391–396. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bozdemir, O. (2016). Language and women. Journal of Education and Training Studies, 4(5), 92–100. https://doi.org/10.11114/jets.v4i5.1265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burck, C. (2011). Living in several languages: Language, gender and identities. European Journal of Women’s Studies, 18(4), 361–378. https://doi.org/10.1177/1350506811415196

  • Cameron, D. (2007). The myth of Mars and Venus: Do men and women really speak different languages. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., Adams, A., Wasco, S., Ahrens, C., & Sefl, T. (2010). “What Has It Been Like for You to Talk With Me Today?”: The impact of participating in interview research on rape survivors. Violence Against Women, 16, 60–83. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801209353576

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L. (1990). Gender, language and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 941–951. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.941

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L, Alawa, L., Lee, Y., Zhao, B., & Kim, E. (2016). Stereotypes about gender and science: Women ≠ scientists. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(2), 244–260. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684315622645

  • Cheryan, S., Siy, J. O., Vichayapai, M., Drury, B. J., & Kim, S. (2011). Do female and male role models who embody STEM stereotypes hinder women’s anticipated success in STEM? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 656–664. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611405218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. (1993). Influence: Science and practice. Harper Collins College Publishers.

  • Clément-Guillotin, C., & Fontayne, P. (2011). Situational malleability of gender schema. Sex Roles, 64(5/6), 426–439. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9912-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dailey, R. M., & Claus, R. E. (2001). The Relationship between interviewer characteristics and physical and sexual abuse disclosures among substance users: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Drug Issues, 31(4), 867–888. https://doi.org/10.1177/002204260103100404

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eakins, B. W., & Eakins, R. G. (1978). Sex differences in human communication. Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, D. G., & McCallister, L. (1980). Relational control sequences in sex-typed and androgynous groups. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 44, 35–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ertel, S. (1986). Language, thought and culture: Toward a mergence of diverging problem fields. In I. Kurcz, G. W. Shugar, & J. H. Danks (Eds.), Knowledge and language (pp. 139–163). Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, B. A. (1983). Differential effects of sexual composition and interactional content on interaction patterns in dyads. Human Communication Research, 9, 225–238. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1983.tb00696

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleason, J. B., & Greif, E. B. (1983). Men’s speech to young children. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender and society (pp. 140–150). Newbury House.

  • Green, R., Sandall, J., & Phelps, C. (2005). Effect of experimenter attire and sex on participant productivity. Social Behavior and Personality, 33(2), 125–132. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2005.33.2.125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, K., & Katz, A. (2006). Metaphor production in online conversation: Gender and friendship status. Discourse Processes, 42(1), 75–98. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp4201_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jastrzębska, J., & Dryll, E. (2008). Autonarracja osób o różnych stylach przywiązania. Wybrane techniki formalnej analizy tekstu [Autonarration of Persons with Different Attachment Styles. Selected Techniques of Formal Text Analysis]. In B. Janusz, K. Gdowska, B. de Barbaro (ed.), Narracja. Teoria i praktyka [Narration. Theory and Practice] (pp. 133–153). Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego.

  • Karaçam, S. (2016). Scientist-image stereotypes: The relationships among their indicators. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 16(3), 1027–1049. https://doi.org/10.12738/estp.2016.3.0005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman’s place. Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. E., & Garretson, M. E. (2003). The effects of interviewer gender and child gender on the informativeness of alleged child sexual abuse victims in forensic interviews. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022595129689

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leathers, D. G. & Eaves M.H. (2016). Successful nonverbal communication. Principles and applications. Routledge.

  • Levon, E. (2015). Integrating intersectionality in language, gender, and sexuality research. Language and Linguistics Compass, 9(7), 295–308. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, A., Zhang, J., Zhang, Y., Li, M., Hong, X., Zheng, D., & Deng, R. (2020). The role of semantic gender in name comprehension: An event-related potentials study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 49(1), 175–185. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09677-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massey-Abernathy, A. R., & Haseltine, E. (2018). Power talk: Communication styles, vocalization rates and dominance. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48, 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9592-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mavisakalyan, A. (2015). Gender in language and gender in employment. Oxford Development Studies, 43(4), 403–424. https://doi.org/10.1080/13600818.2015.1045857

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramirez-Esparza, N., Slatcher, R. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2007). Are women really more talkative than men? Science, 317, 82. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1139940

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milani, T. M. (2015). Language and masculinities: Performances, intersections, and dislocations. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milanowicz, A., & Bokus, B. (2013). Gender and moral judgments: The role of who is speaking to whom. Journal of Gender Studies, 22(4), 423–443. https://doi.org/10.1080/09589236.2012.719314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, M., & McKinnon, M. (2018). “Human” or “objective” faces of science? Gender stereotypes and the representation of scientists in the media. Public Understanding of Science, pp. 963662518801257. Date of Electronic Publication: 2018 Sep 24.

  • Newman, M., Groom, G., & Handelman, L. (2008). Gender differences in language use. Discourse Processes, 45, 211–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638530802073712

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1990). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrębska, M. (2013). Styl mówienia w schizofrenii [Speech Style in Schizophrenia]. Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM.

  • Obrębska, M., Kleka, P. (2016a). Gadatliwość kobiet: prawda czy stereotyp? [Women talkativenesstruth or stereotype?]. In J. Mampe, H. Makurat, Ł. Owczinnikowa, & F. Marzouk (Eds.), Socjolingwistyczne badania w teorii i praktyce. Ujęcie interdyscyplinarne [Sociolinguistic research in theory and practice. Interdisciplinary approach] (pp. 77–86). Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Gdańskiego.

  • Obrębska, M., & Kleka, P. (2016b). Wpływ aktywizacji schematu płci i potrzeby dostosowania interpersonalnego na wybory leksykalne kobiet i mężczyzn [Influence of gender schema activation and need for interpersonal accommodation on lexical choices of men and women]. Psychologia Społeczna, 11(2), 170–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrębski, T., & Stolarski, M. (2006). UAM text toolsA flexible NLP architecture. In N. Calzolari (Ed.), Fifth international conference on language resources and evaluation, Genua, Italy, 24–26.05.2006 (p. 2259–2262). ELRA.

  • Oppermann, K., & Weber, E. (2000). Język kobiet. Język mężczyzn. Jak porozumieć się w miejscu pracy [The language of women. The language of men. How to communicate in the workplace]. Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.

  • Paluchowski, W.J. (2010). Komputerowa analiza narracyjności. Wybrane problemy metodologiczne [Computer analysis of narrativeness. Selected methodological problems]. In M. Straś-Romanowska, B. Bartosz, M. Żurko (Eds.). Badania narracyjne w psychologii [Narrative research in psychology]. Wydawnictwo ENETEIA.

  • Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind: Investigations into the nature of belief systems and personality systems. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter effects in behavioral research. Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S. (1933). The experimental situation as a psychological problem. Psychological Review, 40, 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rzepa, T. (2006). Psychologia komunikowania się dla menedżerów [The psychology of communication for managers]. Wydawnictwo Difin.

  • Smyth, F. L., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). On the gender-science stereotypes held by scientists: Explicit accord with gender-ratios, implicit accord with scientific identity. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00415

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The human pursuit of difference. Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1991). You just don’t understand: Women and men in conversation. Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tislenkova, I., Tikhaeva, V., Bgantseva, I., & Ionkina, E. (2020). Psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic parameters of upper-class ageing women’s speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 49(2), 319–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-020-09692-w

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vetulani, Z., Walczak, B., Obrębski, T., & Vetulani, G. (1988). Unambiguous coding of the inflections of Polish nouns and its applications in electronic dictionaries—format POLEX/Jednoznaczne kodowanie fleksji rzeczownika polskiego i jego zastosowanie w słownikach elektronicznych—format POLEX. Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM.

  • Walczak, B. (2010). Czy język polski dyskryminuje kobiety? [Does Polish discriminate against women?] In J. Arabski & J. Ziębka (Eds.), Płeć języka – język płci [The gender of the language—The language of the gender] (217–228). Wydawnictwo Wyższej Szkoły Zarządzania Marketingowego i Języków Obcych.

  • West, C., & Zimmerman, D. (1987). Doing gender. Gender and Society, 1, 125–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S., Brown, N., Mejia, C., & Lavori, P. (2002). Effects of interviewer characteristics on reported sexual behavior of California Latino couples. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Science, 24, 38–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986302024001003

  • Wojciszke, B. (2013). Psychologia społeczna [Social psychology]. Wydawnictwo Naukowe Scholar.

  • Wood, J. T. (1995). Gendered interaction: Masculine and feminine styles of verbal conversation. In K. S. Verderber (Ed.), Voices: A selection of multicultural readings (pp. 18–39). Wadsworth Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, S., & Besley, J. (2018). Talking aggressively about GMOs? Examining the effect of aggressive risk communication with communicator’s facial expression and gender. Journal of Risk Research, 21(12), 1592–1607. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2017.1351480

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zadykowicz-Skwirosz, A. (2018). Współczesny człowiek nauki, czyli kto? Jak kształtuje się wizerunek naukowca w zależności od wieku, płci i środowiska respondentów [Who is a modern scientist? The image of a scientist depending on the age, sex and background of the interviewees]. Humanistica, 21(2), 278–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Żurko, M. (2008). Wybrane metody analizy i interpretacji narracji autobiograficznej w ujęciu hermeneutycznym [Selected methods of analysis and interpretation of autobiographical narration in a hermeneutical approach]. [In:] B. Janusz, K. Gdowska, B. de Barbaro (Eds.), Narracja. Teoria i praktyka [Narration. Theory and Practice] (pp. 105–115). Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagielońskiego.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Monika Obrębska.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standard

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Human and Animal Rights

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Obrębska, M., Kleka, P. & Knoll, R. Can a Scientist Be a Young, Attractive Woman? The Stereotype of a Scientist and the Lexical Choices of Women and Men. J Psycholinguist Res 51, 577–595 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-022-09870-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-022-09870-y

Keywords

Navigation