Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 30 August 2019
Sec. Sleep Disorders

Positive and Negative Affect Mediate the Influences of a Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Strategy on Sleep Quality

  • 1Directorate of Psychology & Public Health, School of Health and Society, University of Salford, Salford, United Kingdom
  • 2School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Faculty of Science, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom

Positive affect, negative affect, and emotion regulation strategies are related to sleep quality. Emotion regulation can also act as either a protective factor against the development of psychopathologies, or as a risk factor for their development, and therefore may be one mechanism linking mental health and sleep. However, currently it is not known whether affect can mediate the impact of emotion regulation strategy use on sleep quality. An opportunity sample in a healthy population completed the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule providing measures of positive and negative affect, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index providing a measure of sleep quality, and the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire to record habitual use of emotion regulation strategies. Data were analysed using regression and mediation analyses. Negative affect and expressive suppression were positively correlated with PSQI score suggesting that as negative affect and expressive suppression use increased, sleep quality decreased. Positive affect was negatively correlated with PSQI score suggesting that as positive affect increased sleep quality improved. Further, mediation analyses revealed that both positive affect and negative affect mediated the impact of expressive suppression on sleep quality. Moreover, this partial mediation provides the first description that the influences of affect and expressive suppression on sleep quality are at least partially distinct. Targeting improvements in negative affect and effective emotion regulation strategy use may improve the efficacy of interventions aimed at improving sleep quality and the reduction in symptomology in psychopathologies.

Introduction

Sleep is essential for health and is related to cognitive performance, emotion regulation (ER) and quality of life (1). Different ER strategies, broadly defined as conscious or non-conscious efforts to start, stop, or modulate emotions (2), have been shown to protect individuals from developing psychopathologies including anxiety and depression, or act as a risk factor for their development (3). Poor sleep quality is linked to impaired emotional functioning and dysregulated ER which may lead to the development of depressive symptoms (4). Indeed, abnormalities in the function of brain structures and neurotransmitters involved in the sleep–wake cycle and affective disorders overlap [e.g. Refs. (58)].

The precise relationships between sleep quality and affect are currently not fully understood. Alvaro et al. (9) suggest that there is a bidirectional relationship between insomnia and affective disorders, suggesting that insomnia can predict prevalence of anxiety and depression and that in turn anxiety and depression can predict the prevalence of insomnia. Conversely, it has also been suggested that sleep quality can predict positive and negative affect but not vice versa (10). Interestingly, for individuals with insomnia and major depressive disorder, the treatment of insomnia with cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy has been shown to improve depressive symptoms even without simultaneous antidepressant pharmacotherapy (11). Additionally, impaired sleep quality has been shown to influence emotional reactivity to everyday events in healthy individuals and those with minor or major unipolar depression. Adopting an experience sampling method allowing emotional reactivity to daily events to be recorded throughout participants’ daily life, sleep difficulties were associated with enhanced negative affect to unpleasant events and a reduced response to neutral events. However, in individuals with unipolar depression, sleep difficulties were associated with increased negative affect for all everyday events (12).

It is possible that habitual ER strategy use is one mechanism linking mental health and sleep. Cognitive reappraisal, an antecedent-focussed ER strategy, involves the reappraisal of a potentially emotion-eliciting situation in a manner that changes its emotional impact. Expressive suppression is a response-focussed ER strategy that involves inhibiting ongoing emotion-expressive behavior (13). In laboratory paradigms investigating strategies aimed at modifying affect, cognitive reappraisal has been shown to be more effective than expressive suppression [see Ref. ( for a review]. ER has consistently been linked to affect in both clinical and non-clinical populations. Individuals who use maladaptive ER strategies including expressive suppression experience higher levels of internalizing symptoms including depressive and anxiety-related symptoms, whereas individuals who adopt adaptive ER strategies including cognitive reappraisal display fewer symptoms [e.g. Refs. (1518)]. Further, cognitive reappraisal, an adaptive ER strategy, has been shown to offer protection against the development of psychopathologies, while the maladaptive ER strategy, expressive suppression, is linked to increased likelihood of developing an affective disorder [see (3) for a meta-analysis].

The neuroscientific study of ER has predominantly focussed on the investigation of the neural mechanisms underpinning cognitive reappraisal. At the neural level, in healthy individuals, ER has consistently been shown to recruit brain regions involved in top-down cognitive control (e.g. regions of the prefrontal cortex) as well as subcortical limbic structures including the amygdala [for reviews and meta-analyses see Ref. (19, 20)]). Recently, a meta-analysis of patients with mood or anxiety disorders demonstrated abnormal decreases and increases in the activation of brain networks involved in ER (21). Additionally, increased use of ER can predict stronger anatomical connections between the amygdala and PFC, while trait anxiety is negatively correlated with amygdala–PFC connectivity (22). Moreover, increased activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during ER can predict improvements in depressive symptoms in major depressive disorder (23). These findings at both the behavioral and neural level (including both functional and anatomical investigations) demonstrate that ER is linked to affect and psychopathology. Specifically, expressive suppression can act as a risk factor for the development of psychopathologies while cognitive reappraisal offers protection against the development of affective disorders.

Reduced focus has been devoted to the investigation of the links between specific ER strategies and sleep quality. Studies investigating links between expressive suppression, cognitive reappraisal and sleep quality have adopted self-report psychometric questionnaires and laboratory paradigms. Reduced sleep quality has been shown to be related to increased habitual use of expressive suppression, whereas sleep quality was not related to habitual use of cognitive reappraisal (24). However, converging evidence demonstrates that an evening chronotype, commonly associated with reduced sleep duration and quality, particularly in those working morning/regular day shifts (2528), is associated with depression in the general population [e.g. Refs. (29, 30)]. Recently, an evening chronotype was associated with increased habitual use of expressive suppression, while a morning chronotype was associated with increased habitual use of cognitive reappraisal and reduced use of expressive suppression (31). These initial studies adopting psychometric approaches demonstrate links between habitual ER strategy use and sleep quality/circadian profiles. Like the research investigating the neural mechanisms underpinning ER, research adopting laboratory paradigms has focused on the ER strategy cognitive reappraisal, revealing mixed findings. Mauss et al. (32) suggested that poorer sleep quality is associated with lower cognitive reappraisal ability, whereas others have demonstrated that sleep quality scores are not related to cognitive reappraisal ability (33). Findings are also mixed in studies that have employed sleep restriction; sleep restriction has been associated with a decrease in self-reported cognitive reappraisal success (34), but did not impact adolescents’ ability to use cognitive reappraisal (35). Consequently, the exact relationships between ER and sleep quality are not yet fully understood.

The aim of the current study is to investigate the links between affect, ER, and sleep in healthy individuals. We predicted that within a healthy population, habitual use of cognitive reappraisal, a beneficial and protective ER strategy, would predict better sleep quality. In contrast, we hypothesized that habitual use of expressive suppression, a maladaptive ER strategy, would predict poor sleep quality. At present, however, it is not known whether affect can mediate the relationship between ER and sleep quality, and we therefore extend this approach to investigate whether positive and negative affect can mediate the impact of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression on sleep quality. Understanding the links between affect, ER, and sleep quality in healthy populations will help to guide future research on how these processes are altered in clinical populations.

Materials and Methods

Participants

An opportunity sample of 89 undergraduate students (59 female) aged 18–47 years (M = 22.50, SD = 4.54) from the University of Salford participated in this study. Gender and age information were omitted by 7 individuals. Participants read a Participant Information Sheet before providing informed consent via voluntary completion of the questionnaires. Ethical approval was obtained from the School of Health Sciences and School of Health & Society Ethics Committee at University of Salford. Where appropriate participants received course credit for completion of the questionnaires (n = 14).

Design

The study adopted a cross-sectional correlational design with five variables; positive affect, negative affect, cognitive reappraisal, expression suppression, and sleep quality.

Materials

Participants completed self-report questionnaires to record levels of positive and negative affect (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; PANAS) (36), habitual use of ER strategies (Emotion Regulation Scale; ERQ) (13), and sleep quality (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PSQI) (37). The PANAS consists of 20 words used to describe positive and negative feelings and emotions. Participants are required to rate the extent to which a word describes how they feel on a 5-point Likert scale “indicate to what extent you feel this way right now, that is, at the present moment” ranging from 1 (very slightly) to 5 (extremely). The PANAS provides a measure of positive affect (10 items) and negative affect (10 items), with the responses summed for each measure providing a minimum score of 10 (indicative of lower affect) and a maximum score of 50 (indicative of higher affect). Watson et al. (36) report moderately good reliability and validity. Across 6 differing time frames, for both subscales, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient exceeded 0.84 in all instances. Test–retest reliability is 0.68 for positive affect and 0.71 for negative affect. The two subscales have been shown to be independent (r = −0.09).

The ERQ is a 10-item questionnaire that measures respondents’ tendency to regulate their emotions via two different ER strategies; cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression (e.g. “When I’m faced with a stressful situation, I make myself think about it in a way that helps me stay calm”). Responses are provided on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Six items relate to cognitive reappraisal providing a minimum score of 6 (indicating lower use of cognitive reappraisal) and a maximum score of 42 (indicating higher use of cognitive reappraisal). Four items relate to expressive suppression providing a minimum score of 4 (indicating lower use of expressive suppression) and a maximum score of 28 (indicating higher use of expressive suppression). Alpha reliabilities for the ERQ are 0.70 for cognitive reappraisal and 0.73 for expressive suppression. Test–retest reliability was 0.69 for both subscales.

The PSQI is a 19-item questionnaire that measures the patterns and quality of sleep. Questions include 4-point Likert scales for frequency or severity of problem and by reporting times (e.g. “how many hours of actual sleep did you get at night?”). Individuals are instructed that the questions (e.g. “during the past month, how often have you had trouble sleeping because you cannot get to sleep within 30 minutes?”) relate to “usual sleep habits during the past month only. Your answers should indicate the most accurate reply for the majority of days and nights in the past month”. The total score is calculated by summing seven subscale scores providing a minimum possible score of 0 and a maximum possible score of 21. The PQSI has a cutoff score where a total score of < 6 indicates good quality sleep and a score of > 5 indicates poor quality sleep. A Cronbach’s alpha for the PSQI components of 0.83 indicates a high level of internal homogeneity. Further, the PSQI has a diagnostic sensitivity of 89.6% and specificity of 86.5% (kappa = 0.75, p < 0.001) in distinguishing good and poor sleepers.

Procedure

The questionnaires were uploaded onto Bristol Online Surveys, an online platform used to collect survey data. Participants viewed a Participant Information Sheet before completing the questionnaires. Alternatively, participants completed the questionnaires face-to-face by arranging an appointment with the researcher (n = 18). All participants had the opportunity to seek clarification before completing the questionnaires.

Analysis

After checking the data met parametric assumptions (see Results section), relationships between variables were initially analysed using correlational analyses. Where significant relationships were evident, and regression model assumptions had been checked (see Results section), regression and mediation analyses were conducted. This approach allowed the investigation of whether expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal were able to significantly predict sleep quality. The mediation analyses tested whether positive affect and/or negative affect were able to significantly mediate any relationships between habitual emotional regulation strategy use and sleep quality.

Results

Descriptive statistics for the questionnaire measures are presented in Table 1. Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to assess normality suggesting that PSQI scores were not normally distributed, D (89) = 0.119, p = 0.003. Consequently, Spearman’s correlational analyses were conducted to quantify the relationships between emotional factors and sleep quality. PSQI score was positively correlated with expressive suppression, rs (87) = 0.278, p = 0.004, demonstrating that as habitual expressive suppression use increases sleep quality decreases. The relationship between PSQI score and habitual cognitive reappraisal use approached significance, rs (87) = −0.157, p = 0.071. Positive affect was negatively correlated with PSQI score, rs (87) = −0.305, p = 0.002, demonstrating that greater levels of positive affect were related to improved sleep quality. Finally, negative affect was positively correlated with PSQI score, rs (87) = 0.509, p < 0.001, suggesting that lower levels of negative affect were related to improved sleep quality.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for emotional factors and sleep quality.

As significant correlations were present between expressive suppression, positive affect, negative affect, and sleep quality, regression analyses were conducted to test the hypotheses that affect mediates the relationship between expressive suppression and sleep quality (Table 2 and Figure 1). Data met the assumptions relevant for regression analysis. An analysis of standardized residuals demonstrated that the data contained no outliers; standardized residual minimum = −1.903, standardized residual maximum = 2.517. Collinearity was not a concern as all tolerance scores were > 0.878, and all variance inflation factors < 1.139. The data also met the assumption of independent errors as indicated by a Durban-Watson value of 1.675. The histogram of standardized residuals indicated that the data contained approximately normally distributed errors, as did the normal P–P plot of standardized residuals, which showed points were close to the line. The scatterplot of standardized residuals showed that the data met the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and linearity. Finally, the data also met the assumption of non-zero variances with all scores > 16.511.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Positive and negative affect as mediators between expressive suppression and sleep quality.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Partial mediation between expressive suppression and sleep quality by both (A) negative affect, and (B) positive affect. Data values are regression betas for specific pathways, with standard errors shown in parentheses. High PSQI scores indicate poor sleep. *p < .05, **p < .01.

In model 1, standardized regression betas indicated that expressive suppression was a significant predictor of negative affect, β = 0.210, SE = 0.184, p = 0.043 (Figure 1A, path a), and that negative affect significantly predicted PSQI score, β = .465, SE = 0.041, p < 0.001 (Figure 1A, path b). Irrespective of affect, the total effect of expressive suppression on sleep quality was significant, β = 0.310, SE = 0.100, p = 0.004 (Figure 1A, path c). When controlling for negative affect, expressive suppression still predicted PSQI score, β = .212, SE = 0.072, p = 0.020 (Figure 1A, path c’), consistent with partial mediation. Approximately 30% of the variance in sleep quality was accounted for by the predictors (R2 = 0.303). The indirect mediated path (Figure 1A, path c–c’) was tested using a bootstrap estimation approach with 1,000 samples. This result indicated that the indirect coefficient was significant, β = −0.098, SE = 0.048, p = 0.018, 95% CI = −0.184, −0.028, confirming mediation. Together, these analyses reveal that negative affect partially mediates the effect of expressive suppression on sleep quality (Table 2).

Model 2 indicated that expressive suppression was a significant predictor of positive affect, β = −0.297, SE = 0.166, p = 0.004, (Figure 1B path a), and that positive affect significantly predicted PSQI score, β = −0.244, SE = 0.050, p = 0.018, (Figure 1B, path b). Irrespective of affect, the total effect of expressive suppression on sleep quality was significant, β = 0.310, SE = 0.100, p = 0.004 (Figure 1A, path c). When controlling for positive affect, expressive suppression still predicted PSQI score, β = .238, SE = 0.082, p = 0.021 (Figure 1B, path c’), consistent with partial mediation. Approximately 15% of the variance in sleep quality was accounted for by the predictors (R2 = 0.151). The indirect mediated path (Figure 1B, path c–c’) was tested using a bootstrap estimation approach with 1,000 samples, demonstrating that the indirect coefficient was significant, β = 0.073, SE = 0.040, p = 0.010, 95% CI = −0.162, −0.024, confirming mediation. Together, these analyses reveal that positive affect partially mediates the effect of expressive suppression on sleep quality (Figure 1).

Discussion

To our knowledge the current study is the first to investigate if affect mediates the relationship between habitual ER use and sleep quality in a healthy population. Correlational analyses demonstrated that increased positive affect was associated with improved sleep quality, whereas greater negative affect scores were associated with reduced sleep quality. Increased use of expressive suppression was also associated with poorer sleep quality, while the relationship between cognitive reappraisal and sleep quality was non-significant. Regression and mediation analyses revealed that increased habitual use of expressive suppression, theorized to be a maladaptive ER strategy, was shown to predict poorer sleep quality. Finally, both positive and negative affect were found to mediate the effects of expressive suppression on sleep quality.

The finding that expressive suppression predicts poorer sleep quality is consistent with recent work in an adolescent sample (38) and adult population (24), but here we extend this using mediation analysis to report the novel finding that both positive and negative affect influence the effect of expressive suppression on sleep quality. Moreover, we demonstrate that negative affect explains more of the variance in sleep quality than positive affect. Previously, using structural equation modelling, it has been shown that while negative affect was able to predict sleep quality, positive affect was not (39). Here, however, we do report a significant influence of positive affect on sleep quality, but consistent with this previous work, negative affect had a more prominent influence. This observation suggests that therapies targeting the regulation of negative affect may be more successful in improving sleep quality compared to interventions aimed at improving positive affect. Further research will be necessary to elucidate the specific negative affect factors responsible for this relationship. For example, negative affect is associated with the experience of anxiety, fear, sadness, depression, and stress (40, 41). While general negative affect is strongly linked to affective disorders, it often has differential relationships with specific psychopathologies (42). Thus, it will be important to investigate which specific secondary level negative affect factors (e.g. anxiety, fear, sadness, depression, or stress) influence the relationship between ER strategy use and sleep quality. Such research may help to develop more efficacious interventions. The observation that cognitive reappraisal is not significantly correlated with sleep quality or affect is surprising given that this ER strategy has been shown to offer protection against the development of psychopathologies. Nonetheless, this observation is supported by recent work suggesting that cognitive reappraisal is not correlated with sleep quality (24). However, it should be noted that the correlation between cognitive reappraisal and sleep quality approached significance (p = 0.071), and further studies are needed to explore the relationships between cognitive reappraisal and sleep quality in healthy and clinical populations.

Sleep disturbances and psychopathologies are often comorbid. It has been suggested both that treatments aimed at reducing negative affect may improve the effectiveness of interventions for insomnia, and that the efficacy of interventions aimed at preventing or treating affective disorders may be enhanced by simultaneously addressing sleep quality (39). The current findings in healthy individuals support this claim. Recently, emotion regulation therapy has shown reductions in symptom severity for generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder patients (43). Moreover, functional changes in brain regions located within the salience network and default mode network, both of which show disruptions in depression and anxiety patients, have shown treatment-related changes following emotion regulation therapy corresponding with clinical improvements (44). Our current research indicates that future studies evaluating the potential benefits of emotion regulation therapy should simultaneously assess improvements in sleep quality and changes in psychopathology.

The current research presents novel findings and furthers our understanding of the relationships between ER, affect, and sleep quality. However, a few limitations warrant consideration. The study adopted a cross-sectional correlational design preventing causal inferences regarding the nature of the relationships between ER, affect, and sleep quality. Consequently, experimental, longitudinal and intervention studies are required to provide such causal inferences. Additionally, the study employed a convenience sample of healthy individuals, and so caution must be taken when applying these findings to clinical populations. Nonetheless, the findings reported here provide a basis for similar research within clinical groups. The study did not employ any inclusion or exclusion criteria, such as screening for psychological conditions or sleep disorders, and so it is possible that other variables could mediate the relationships between ER and sleep quality. Demographic variables such as age and gender were not identified on an a priori basis as variables of interest and so were omitted from the analytical framework. Sleep quality has been shown to reduce with age [e.g. Ref. (45)], while age- and gender-related differences in ER strategies are evident [e.g. Ref. (4648)]. Studies including these demographic variables on an a priori basis are needed to investigate if age and gender influence the relationships between ER, affect and sleep quality.

Additionally, while the current study used established psychometric measures for assessing ER, affect and sleep quality, objective measures were not included. For instance, actigraphy, an objective measure of sleep, is not always correlated with self-report measures of sleep quality provided by the PSQI (49). Further, negative affect has been shown to be related to self-reported sleep but not objective measures of sleep (50). In the current study habitual ER strategy use was assessed via self-reports. Future studies can seek to assess ER using laboratory paradigms. Moreover, recently it has been argued that ER paradigms should also investigate the decision to deploy ER strategies, rather than using paradigms that only investigate instructed ER (51, 52). Adopting such approaches permits the investigation of individual differences in the decisions to use ER and will aid development of our knowledge of traits that can be targeted for clinical intervention.

The novel findings reported here in a healthy population provide the basis for progression to similar work in clinical populations with diagnosed sleep disturbances and/or affective disorders. Future research, utilizing clinical populations, will develop our understanding of the relationships between ER, affect and sleep quality and raise the possibility of potential new therapeutic tools for the treatment of both psychopathologies and sleep disorders.

Adopting mediation analyses, positive and negative affect were found to partially mediate the relationship between expressive suppression and sleep quality. These data suggest that including elements aimed at modulating affect, and in particular reducing negative affect, as well as minimizing habitual use of expressive suppression may improve the effectiveness of psychological interventions for patients with sleep disorders.

Data Availability

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of School of Health Sciences and School of Health & Society Ethics Committee at University of Salford. Informed consent was inferred via voluntary completion of the questionnaires. The protocol was approved by the School of Health Sciences and School of Health & Society Ethics Committee at University of Salford.

Author Contributions

IL: data collection. IL and RB study design. IL, RB and AH statistical analysis, manuscript preparation, interpretation of results. All authors agreed final manuscript submission.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, Alessi C, Bruni O, DonCarlos L, et al. National sleep foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: final report. Sleep Heal (2015) 1:233–43. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Gross JJ. Emotion Regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol Inq (2015) 26:1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev (2010) 30:217–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. O’Leary K, Bylsma LM, Rottenberg J. Why might poor sleep quality lead to depression? A role for emotion regulation. Cogn Emot (2017a) 31:1698–706. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1247035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Abnormal behavioral phenotypes of serotonin transporter knockout mice: parrellels with human anxiety and depression. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 54:953–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science (1996) 274:1527–31. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Nestler EJ, Carlezon WAJ. The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biol Psychiatry (2006) 59:1151–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ouellet M-C, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Morin CM. Sleep-wake disturbances after traumatic brain injury. The Lancet Neurol (2015) 14(7):746–57. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00068-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Alvaro PK, Roberts RM, Harris JK. a systematic review assessing bidirectionality between sleep disturbances, anxiety, and depression. Sleep (2013) 36:1059–68. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bouwmans MEJ, Bos EH, Hoenders HJR, Oldehinkel AJ, de Jonge P. Sleep quality predicts positive and negative affect but not vice versa. An electronic diary study in depressed and healthy individuals. J Affect Disord (2017) 207:260–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Carney CE, Edinger JD, Kuchibhatla M, Lachowski AM, Bogouslavsky O, Krystal AD, et al. Cognitive behavioral insomnia therapy for those with insomnia and depression: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Sleep (2017) 40(4):1–13. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. O’Leary K, Small BJ, Panaite V, Bylsma LM, Rottenberg J. Sleep quality in healthy and mood-disordered persons predicts daily life emotional reactivity. Cogn Emot (2017b) 31:435–43. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1126554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol (2003) 85:348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Webb TL, Miles E, Sheeran P. Dealing with feelings: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychol Bull (2012) 138:775–808. doi: 10.1037/a0027600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Li YI, Starr LR, Hershenberg R. Savoring and dampening in daily life: positive rumination moderates the association between daily events and depressive symptoms. J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2018) 35(6):583–91. doi: 10.1007/s1086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Aldao A, Dixon-Gordon KL. Broadening the scope of research on emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology. Cogn Behav Ther. (2014) 43(1):22–33. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2013.816769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. McMahon TP, Naragon-Gainey K. The moderating effect of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies on reappraisal: a daily diary study. Cogn Ther Res (2018) 42(5):552–64. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9913-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. McMahon TP, Naragon-Gainey K. The multilevel structure of daily emotion regulation-strategy use: an examination of within- and between-person associations in naturalistic settings. Clin Psychol Sci (2019) 7(2):321–39. doi: 10.1177/2167702618807408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2012) 1251:E1–E24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex, (2014) 24:2981–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Pico-Perez M, Radua J, Steward T, Menchon JM, Soriano-Mas C. Emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of fMRI cognitive reappraisal studies. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2017) 79:96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Eden AS, Schreiber J, Anwander A, Keuper K, Laeger I, Zwanzger P, et al. Emotion regulation and trait anxiety arepredicted by the microstructure of fibers between amygdala and prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci (2015) 35:6020–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3659-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Heller AS, Johnstone T, Peterson MJ, Kolden GG, Kalin NH, Davidson RJ. Increased prefrontal cortex activity during negative emotion regulation as a predictor of depression symptom severity trajectory over 6 months. JAMA Psychiatry (2013) 70:1181–9. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ellis EM, Prather AA, Grenen EG, Ferrer RA. Direct and indirect associations of cognitive reappraisal and suppression with disease biomarkers. Psychol Health (2019) 34(3):336–54. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2018.1529313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dmitrzak-Węglarz M, Pawlak J, Wilkosc M, Miechowicz I, Maciukiewicz M, Ciarkowska W, et al. Chronotype and sleep quality as a subphenotype in association studies of clock genes in mood disorders. Acta Neurobiol Exp (2016) 76:32–42. doi: 10.21307/ane-2017-003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lima ÁMA, Varela GCG, Silveira HAC, Parente RDG, Araujo JF. Evening chronotypes experience poor sleep quality when taking classes with early starting times. Sleep Sci (2010) 3(1):45–8.

Google Scholar

27. Roeser K, Meule A, Schwerdtle B, Kübler A, Schlarb AA. Subjective sleep quality exclusively mediates the relationship between morningness-eveningness preference and self-perceived stress response. Chronobiol Int (2012) 29(7):955–60. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.699124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Juda M, Vetter C, Roenneberg T. Chronotype modulates sleep duration, sleep quality, and social jet lag in shift-workers. J Biol Rhythms (2013) 28(2):141–51. doi: 10.1177/0748730412475042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Levandovski R, Dantas G, Fernandes LC, Caumo W, Torres I, Roenneberg T, et al. Depression scores associate with chronotype and social jetlag in a rural population. Chronobiol Int (2011) 28(9):771–8. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.602445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Merikanto I, Lahti T, Kronholm E, Peltonen M, Laatikainen T, Vartiainen E, et al. Evening types are prone to depression. Chronobiol Int (2013) 30(5):719–25. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.784770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Watts AL, Norbury R. Reduced effective emotion regulation in night owls. J Biol Rhythms (2017) 32(4):369–75. doi: 10.1177/0748730417709111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Mauss IB, Troy AS, LeBourgeois MK. Poorer sleep quality is associated with lower emotion-regulation ability in a laboratory paradigm. Cogn Emot (2013) 27(3):567–76. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.727783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Minkel JD, McNealy K, Gianaros PJ, Drabant EM, Gross JJ, Manuck SB, et al. Sleep quality and neural circuit function supporting emotion regulation. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord (2012) 7(2):22. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-2-22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Tamm S, Nilsonne G, Schwarz J, Golkar A, Kecklund G, Petrovic P, et al. Sleep restriction caused impaired emotional regulation without detectable brain activation changes-a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. R Soc Open Sci (2019) 6(3):181704. doi: 10.1098/rsos.181704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Reddy R, Palmer CA, Jackson C, Farris SG, Alfano CA. Impact of sleep restriction versus idealized sleep on emotional experience, reactivity and regulation in healthy adolescents. J Sleep Res (2017) 26(4):516–25. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Watson D, Clark L, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol (1988a) 54(6), 1063–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res (1989) 28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Palmer CA, Oosterhoff B, Bower JL, Kaplow JB, Alfano CA. Associations among adolescent sleep problems, emotion regulation, and affective disorders: findings from a nationally representative sample. J Psychiatr Res (2018) 96:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Stewart JC, Rand KL, Hawkins MAW, Stines JA. Associations of the shared and unique aspects of positive and negative emotional factors with sleep quality. Pers Individ Differ (2011) 50:609–14. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Watson D, Clark LA, Carey G. Positive and negative affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. J Abnormal Psychol (1988b) 97:346. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.3.346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther (1995) 33:335–43. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Paulus DJ, Talkovsky AM, Heggeness LF, Norton PJ. Beyond negative affectivity: a hierarchical model of global and transdiagnostic vulnerabilities for emotional disorders. Cogn Behav Ther (2015) 44(5):389–405. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2015.1017529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Renna ME, Quintero JM, Soffer A, Pino M, Ader L, Fresco DM, et al. A pilot study of emotion regulation therapy for generalized anxiety and depression: findings from a diverse sample of young adults. Behav Ther (2018) 49:403–18. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Scult MA, Fresco DM, Gunning FM, Liston C, Seeley SH, Garcia E, et al. Changes in functional connectivity following treatment with emotion regulation therapy. Front Behav Neurosci (2019) 13(10), 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Gadie A, Shafto M, Leng Y, Cam-CAN, Kievit RA. How are age-related differences in sleep quality associated with health outcomes? An epidemiological investigation in a UK cohort of 2406 adults. BMJ Open (2017) 2017 7:e014920. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Schirda B, Valentine TR, Aldao A, Prakash RS. Age-related differences in emotion regulation strategies: examining the role of contextual factors. Dev Psychol (2016) 52(9):1370–80. doi: 10.1037/dev0000194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Aldao A. Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Pers Individ Differ (2011) 51(6):704–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. McRae K, Ochsner KN, Mauss IB, Gabrieli JJD, Gross JJ. Gender differences in emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Process Intergroup Relat (2008) 11(2):143–62. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Grandner MA, Kripkem DF, Yoon I-Y, Youngstedt SD. Criterion validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: investigation in a non-clinical sample. Sleep Biol Rhythyms (2006) 4(2):129–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2006.00207.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Scott AJ, Rowse G, Webb TL. A structural equation model of the relationship between insomnia, negative affect, and paranoid thinking. PLoS ONE (2017) 12(10):e0186233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Doré BP, Weber J, Ochsner KN. Neural predictors of decisions to cognitively control emotion. J Neurosci (2017) 37:2580–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2526-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bendall RCA. The neural mechanisms able to predict future emotion regulation decisions. J Neurophysiol (2017) 118:1824–7. doi: 10.1152/jn.00397.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep, expressive suppression, cognitive reappraisal, emotion, mental health, psychopathology, emotion regulation, mediation analysis

Citation: Latif I, Hughes ATL and Bendall RCA (2019) Positive and Negative Affect Mediate the Influences of a Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Strategy on Sleep Quality. Front. Psychiatry 10:628. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00628

Received: 11 March 2019; Accepted: 05 August 2019;
Published: 30 August 2019.

Edited by:

Lino Nobili, University of Genoa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Amanda Richdale, La Trobe University, Australia
Daniela Tempesta, University of L’Aquila, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Latif, Hughes and Bendall. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robert C. A. Bendall, r.c.a.bendall@salford.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.